Hide metadata

dc.date.accessioned2018-08-16T10:39:58Z
dc.date.available2018-08-16T10:39:58Z
dc.date.created2017-03-06T09:26:57Z
dc.date.issued2017
dc.identifier.citationTeigen, Karl Halvor Böhm, Gisela Bruckmüller, Susanne Hegarty, Peter Luminet, Olivier . Long live the king! Beginnings loom larger than endings of past and recurrent events.. Cognition. 2017, 163, 26-41
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10852/62999
dc.description.abstractEvents are temporal “figures”, which can be defined as identifiable segments in time, bounded by beginnings and endings. But the functions and importance of these two boundaries differ. We argue that beginnings loom larger than endings by attracting more attention, being judged as more important and interesting, warranting more explanation, and having more causal power. This difference follows from a lay notion that additions (the introduction of something new) imply more change and demand more effort than do subtractions (returning to a previous state of affairs). This “beginning advantage” is demonstrated in eight studies of people’s representations of epochs and events on a historical timeline as well as in cyclical change in the annual seasons. People think it is more important to know when wars and reigns started than when they ended, and are more interested in reading about beginnings than endings of historical movements. Transitional events (such as elections and passages from one season to the next) claim more interest and grow in importance when framed as beginnings of what follows than as conclusions of what came before. As beginnings are often identified in retrospect, the beginning advantage may distort and exaggerate their actual historical importance.en_US
dc.languageEN
dc.publisherElsevier Science
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/
dc.titleLong live the king! Beginnings loom larger than endings of past and recurrent events.en_US
dc.typeJournal articleen_US
dc.creator.authorTeigen, Karl Halvor
dc.creator.authorBöhm, Gisela
dc.creator.authorBruckmüller, Susanne
dc.creator.authorHegarty, Peter
dc.creator.authorLuminet, Olivier
cristin.unitcode185,17,5,0
cristin.unitnamePsykologisk institutt
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextpostprint
cristin.qualitycode2
dc.identifier.cristin1455869
dc.identifier.bibliographiccitationinfo:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:ctx&ctx_ver=Z39.88-2004&rft_val_fmt=info:ofi/fmt:kev:mtx:journal&rft.jtitle=Cognition&rft.volume=163&rft.spage=26&rft.date=2017
dc.identifier.jtitleCognition
dc.identifier.volume163
dc.identifier.startpage26
dc.identifier.endpage41
dc.identifier.doihttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.02.013
dc.identifier.urnURN:NBN:no-65570
dc.type.documentTidsskriftartikkelen_US
dc.type.peerreviewedPeer reviewed
dc.source.issn0010-0277
dc.identifier.fulltextFulltext https://www.duo.uio.no/bitstream/handle/10852/62999/1/Beginnings_Cognition_FinalR.pdf
dc.type.versionAcceptedVersion
dc.relation.projectCOST/COST Action IS 1205


Files in this item

Appears in the following Collection

Hide metadata

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International
This item's license is: Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International